ImageVerifierCode 换一换
格式:PDF , 页数:8 ,大小:594.43KB ,
资源ID:454699      下载积分:10000 积分
快捷下载
登录下载
邮箱/手机:
温馨提示:
如需开发票,请勿充值!快捷下载时,用户名和密码都是您填写的邮箱或者手机号,方便查询和重复下载(系统自动生成)。
如填写123,账号就是123,密码也是123。
特别说明:
请自助下载,系统不会自动发送文件的哦; 如果您已付费,想二次下载,请登录后访问:我的下载记录
支付方式: 支付宝扫码支付 微信扫码支付   
注意:如需开发票,请勿充值!
验证码:   换一换

加入VIP,免费下载
 

温馨提示:由于个人手机设置不同,如果发现不能下载,请复制以下地址【http://www.mydoc123.com/d-454699.html】到电脑端继续下载(重复下载不扣费)。

已注册用户请登录:
账号:
密码:
验证码:   换一换
  忘记密码?
三方登录: 微信登录  

下载须知

1: 本站所有资源如无特殊说明,都需要本地电脑安装OFFICE2007和PDF阅读器。
2: 试题试卷类文档,如果标题没有明确说明有答案则都视为没有答案,请知晓。
3: 文件的所有权益归上传用户所有。
4. 未经权益所有人同意不得将文件中的内容挪作商业或盈利用途。
5. 本站仅提供交流平台,并不能对任何下载内容负责。
6. 下载文件中如有侵权或不适当内容,请与我们联系,我们立即纠正。
7. 本站不保证下载资源的准确性、安全性和完整性, 同时也不承担用户因使用这些下载资源对自己和他人造成任何形式的伤害或损失。

版权提示 | 免责声明

本文(ASHRAE FUNDAMENTALS IP CH 12-2013 Odors.pdf)为本站会员(figureissue185)主动上传,麦多课文库仅提供信息存储空间,仅对用户上传内容的表现方式做保护处理,对上载内容本身不做任何修改或编辑。 若此文所含内容侵犯了您的版权或隐私,请立即通知麦多课文库(发送邮件至master@mydoc123.com或直接QQ联系客服),我们立即给予删除!

ASHRAE FUNDAMENTALS IP CH 12-2013 Odors.pdf

1、12.1CHAPTER 12ODORSOdor Sources 12.1Sense of Smell 12.1Factors Affecting Odor Perception. 12.2Odor Sensation Attributes 12.3Dilution of Odors by Ventilation 12.5Odor Concentration . 12.5Olf Units. 12.6ARIOUS factors make odor control an important considerationVin ventilation engineering: (1) contemp

2、orary construction meth-ods result in buildings that allow less air infiltration through the build-ing envelope; (2) indoor sources of odors associated with modernbuilding materials, furnishings, and office equipment have increased;(3) outdoor air is often polluted; and (4) energy costs encourage lo

3、werventilation rates at a time when requirements for a relatively odor-freeenvironment are greater than ever.Since Yaglou et al.s (1936) classic studies, the philosophy behindventilation of nonindustrial buildings has mainly been to provide in-door air that is acceptable to occupants. Air is evaluat

4、ed by the ol-factory sense, although the general chemical sense, which issensitive to irritants in the air, also plays a role.This chapter reviews how odoriferous substances are perceived.Chapter 46 of the 2011 ASHRAE HandbookHVAC Applicationscovers control methods. Chapter 10 of this volume has mor

5、e infor-mation on indoor environmental health.ODOR SOURCESOutdoor sources of odors include automotive and diesel exhausts,hazardous waste sites, sewage treatment plants, compost piles,refuse facilities, printing plants, refineries, chemical plants, andmany other stationary and mobile sources. These

6、sources produceboth inorganic compounds (e.g., ammonia and hydrogen sulfide)and volatile organic compounds (VOCs), including some that evap-orate from solid or liquid particulate matter. Odors emitted by out-door sources eventually enter the indoor environment.Indoor sources also emit odors. Sources

7、 include tobacco prod-ucts, bathrooms and toilets, building materials (e.g., adhesives,paints, caulks, processed wood, carpets, plastic sheeting, insulationboard), consumer products (e.g., food, toiletries, cleaning materials,polishes), hobby materials, fabrics, and foam cushions. In offices,offset

8、printing processes, copiers, and computer printers may pro-duce odors. Electrostatic processes may emit ozone. Humans emit awide range of odorants, including acetaldehyde, ammonia, ethanol,hydrogen sulfide, and mercaptans.Mildew and other decay processes often produce odors in occu-pied spaces (home

9、 and office), damp basements, and ventilationsystems (e.g., from wetted air-conditioning coils and spray dehu-midifiers).Chapter 46 of the 2011 ASHRAE HandbookHVAC Applica-tions gives further information on contaminant sources and genera-tion rates.SENSE OF SMELLOlfactory StimuliOrganic substances w

10、ith molecular weights greater than 300 aregenerally odorless. Some substances with molecular weights less than300 are such potent olfactory stimuli that they can be perceived atconcentrations too low to be detected with direct-reading instruments.Trimethylamine, for example, can be recognized as a f

11、ishy odor by ahuman at a concentration of about 104ppm.Table 1 shows odor detection threshold concentrations forselected compounds. The threshold limit value (TLV) is the con-centration of a compound that should have no adverse health conse-quences if a worker is regularly exposed for 8 h periods (A

12、CGIH,revised annually). Table 1 also includes the ratio of the TLV to theodor threshold for each compound. For ratios greater than 1, mostoccupants can detect the odor and leave the area long before thecompound becomes a health risk. As the ratio increases, the safetyfactor provided by the odor also

13、 increases. Table 1 is not a compre-hensive list of the chemicals found in indoor air. AIHA (1989) andEPA (1992) list odor thresholds for selected chemicals.Olfactory sensitivity often makes it possible to detect potentiallyharmful substances at concentrations below dangerous levels so thatthey can

14、be eliminated. Foul-smelling air is often assumed to beunhealthy. In reality, however, there is little correlation between odorperception and toxicity, and there is considerable individual varia-tion in the perception of pleasantness/unpleasantness of odors.When symptoms such as nausea, headache, an

15、d loss of appetite arecaused by an unpleasant odor, it may not matter whether the air istoxic but whether the odor is perceived to be unpleasant, associatedThe preparation of this chapter is assigned to TC 2.3, Gaseous Air Contam-inants and Gas Contaminant Removal Equipment.Table 1 Odor Thresholds,

16、ACGIH TLVs, and TLV/Threshold Ratios of Selected Gaseous Air PollutantsCompoundOdor Threshold,appmvTLV,bppmv RatioAcetaldehyde 0.067 25-C 360Acetone 62 500 8.1Acetonitrile 1600 20 0.013Acrolein 1.8 0.1-C 0.06Ammonia 17 25 1.5Benzene 61 0.5 0.01Benzyl chloride 0.041 1 24Carbon tetrachloride 250 5 0.0

17、2Chlorine 0.08 0.5 6Chloroform 192 10 0.05Dioxane 12 20 1.7Ethylene dichloride 26 10 0.4Hydrogen sulfide 0.0094 10 1064Methanol 160 200 1.25Methylene chloride 160 50 0.3Methyl ethyl ketone 16 200 12.5Phenol 0.06 5 83Sulfur dioxide 2.7 2 0.74Tetrachloroethane 7.3 1 0.14Tetrachloroethylene 47 25 0.5To

18、luene 1.6 20 13Trichloroethylene 82 10 0.1Xylene (isomers) 20 100 5Sources: ACGIH (updated annually), AIHA (1989).aAll thresholds are detection thresholds (ED50).bAll TLVs are 8 h time-weighted averages, except those shown with -C, which are 15 min ceiling values.12.2 2013 ASHRAE HandbookFundamental

19、swith an unpleasant experience, or simply felt to be out of appropri-ate context. The magnitude of the symptoms is related to the magni-tude of the odor, but even a room with a low but recognizable odorcan make occupants uneasy. Several papers review sensory irritationand its relation to indoor air

20、pollution (Cain and Cometto-Muiz1995; Cometto-Muiz and Cain 1992; Cometto-Muiz et al. 1997;Shams Esfandabad 1993).Anatomy and PhysiologyThe olfactory receptors lie in the olfactory cleft, which is high inthe nasal cavity. About five million olfactory neurons (a small clusterof nerve cells inside the

21、 nasal cavity above the bridge of the nose)each send an axon (an extension of the neuron) into the olfactory bulbof the brain. Information received from the receptors is passed tovarious central structures of the brain (e.g., olfactory cortex, hippo-campus, amygdala). One sniff of an odorant can oft

22、en evoke a com-plex, emotion-laden memory, such as a scene from childhood.The surrounding nasal tissue contains other diffusely distributednerve endings of the trigeminal nerve that also respond to airbornevapors. These receptors mediate the chemosensory responses suchas tickling, burning, cooling,

23、and, occasionally, painful sensationsthat accompany olfactory sensations. Most odorous substances atsufficient concentration also stimulate these nerve endings.Olfactory AcuityThe olfactory acuity of the population is normally distributed.Most people have an average ability to smell substances or to

24、respond to odoriferous stimuli, a few people are very sensitive orhypersensitive, and a few others are insensitive, including some whoare totally unable to smell (anosmic). The olfactory acuity of anindividual varies with the odorant.Hormonal factors, which often influence emotional states, canmodul

25、ate olfactory sensitivity. Although the evidence is not uni-formly compelling, research has found that (1) the sensitivity offemales varies during the menstrual cycle, reaching a peak justbefore and during ovulation (Schneider 1974); (2) females are gen-erally more sensitive than males, but this dif

26、ference only emergesaround the time of sexual maturity (Koelega and Koster 1974);(3) sensitivity is altered by some diseases (Schneider 1974); and(4) various hormones and drugs (e.g., estrogen, alcohol) alter sensi-tivity (Engen et al. 1975; Schneider 1974).Other factors that may affect olfactory pe

27、rception include theindividuals olfactory acuity, the magnitude of flow rate towardolfactory receptors, temperature, and relative humidity. Olfactoryacuity can also vary with age (Stevens et al. 1989; Wysocki and Gil-bert 1989), genetics (Wysocki and Beauchamp 1984), exposure his-tory (Dalton and Wy

28、socki 1996; Wysocki et al. 1997), and diseaseor injury (Cowart et al. 1993, 1997). Humans are able to perceive alarge number of odors, yet untrained individuals are able to nameonly a few (Ruth 1986).Individuals who are totally unable to detect odors are relativelyrare (Cowart et al. 1997). A more c

29、ommon occurrence is an inabilityto detect one or a very limited number of odors, a condition knownas specific anosmia. Although the huge number of possible chem-icals makes for an untestable hypothesis, it has been posited thatmost, if not all, individuals have a specific anosmia to one or morecompo

30、unds (Wysocki and Beauchamp 1984). The fact that individ-uals with specific anosmias have normal olfactory acuity for allother odors suggests that such anosmias may be caused by geneticdifferences.In olfactory science, adaptation refers to decreased sensitivity orresponsiveness to an odor after prol

31、onged exposure. This exposurecan selectively impair the perception of the exposure odorant, butthere are also examples of cross-adaptation, where exposure to oneodorant can result in adaptation to other odors as well. Adaptationcan occur in the short term, where perception of a rooms odor beginsto f

32、ade within seconds of entering the room (Cometto-Muiz andCain 1995; Pierce et al. 1996). With long-term adaptation, an indi-vidual who habitually returns to the same environment does notsmell odors that are quite discernible to a naive observer. This effectappears to shift both the threshold and the

33、 suprathreshold (stimuliabove the threshold level) response to the odor (Dalton and Wysocki1996). This is an important phenomenon for indoor air quality (IAQ)personnel to be aware of because it is often one of the biggest reasonsfor variations in detectability or response in real-world environmentsa

34、nd makes the choice of test population or panelists for air qualityevaluations a critical one.FACTORS AFFECTING ODOR PERCEPTIONHumidity and TemperatureTemperature and humidity can both affect the perception ofodors. Cain et al. (1983) reported that a combination of high tem-perature (78F) and high h

35、umidity exacerbates odor problems.Berglund and Cain (1989) found that air was generally perceived tobe fresher and less stuffy with decreasing temperature and humidity.Fang et al. (1998a, 1998b) and Toftum et al. (1998) found little or noincrease in odor intensity with increasing enthalpy (temperatu

36、re andhumidity), but reported a very significant decrease in odor accept-ability with increasing enthalpy.Not all researchers have supported these findings. Kerka andHumphreys (1956) reported a decrease in odor intensity with increas-ing humidity. Berg-Munch and Fanger (1982) found no increase inodo

37、r intensity with increasing temperature (73.5 to 89.5F). Clausenet al. (1985) found no significant change in odor intensity withincreasing relative humidity (30% to 80%).Although the findings are not homogeneous, they do show thattemperature and humidity can act together to affect ones perceptionof

38、odors. Air that is cooler and drier is generally perceived to befresher and more acceptable even if odor intensity is not affected.Sorption and Release of OdorsBecause furnishings and interior surfaces absorb (and laterdesorb) odors during occupancy, spaces frequently retain normaloccupancy odor lev

39、els long after occupancy has ceased. This isobserved when furnaces or radiators, after a long shutdown, areheated at winter start-up and when evaporator coils warm up. Therate of desorption can be decreased by decreasing temperature andrelative humidity, and increased (as for cleaning) by the revers

40、e.Environmental tobacco smoke may desorb from surfaces longafter smoking has taken place. This phenomenon has caused manyhotels to establish nonsmoking rooms.Where the odor source is intrinsic to the materials (as in lino-leum, paint, rubber, and upholstery), reducing the relative humiditydecreases

41、the rate of odor release. Quantitative values should not beused without considering the sorption/desorption phenomenon.Emotional Responses to OdorsThere can be considerable variation between individuals regard-ing the perceived pleasantness or unpleasantness of a given odor.Responses to odors may be

42、 determined by prior experiences andcan include strong emotional reactions. This is because one of thebrain structures involved in the sense of smell is the amygdala, aregulator of emotional behaviors (Frey 1995). Some IAQ com-plaints can involve emotional responses completely out of propor-tion to

43、the concentration of the odorant or the intensity of the odorit produces.Two theories describe physiological reasons for these strongresponses. One of these is kindling, in which repeated, intermittentstimuli amplify nerve responses. The other is response facilitation,in which an initial stimulus pe

44、rceived as strong is facilitated(becomes greater) rather than adapted to (Frey 1995).Odors 12.3Because of this emotional aspect, IAQ complaints involvingodors can be very difficult to solve, especially if they are comingfrom a few sensitized individuals. It is important to respond quicklyto complain

45、ts to minimize the risk of kindling or response facilita-tion.ODOR SENSATION ATTRIBUTESOdor sensation has four components or attributes: detectability,intensity, character, and hedonic tone.Detectability (or threshold) is the minimum concentration of anodorant that provokes detection by some predete

46、rmined segment ofthe population. Two types of thresholds exist: detection and recog-nition.The detection threshold is the lowest level that elicits responseby a segment of the population. If that segment is 50%, the detectionthreshold is denoted by ED50. Recognition threshold is the lowestlevel at w

47、hich a segment of the population can recognize a givenodor. Thresholds can be attributed to 100%, which includes allolfactory sensitivities, or to 10%, which includes only the most sen-sitive segment of the population. Threshold values are not physicalconstants, but statistical measurements of best

48、estimates.Intensity is a quantitative aspect of a descriptive analysis, statingthe degree or magnitude of the perception elicited. Intensity of theperceived odor is, therefore, the strength of the odoriferous sensa-tion. Detection threshold values and, most often, odor intensitydetermine the need fo

49、r indoor odor controls.Character defines the odor as similar to some familiar smell(e.g., fishy, sour, flowery). Hedonics, or the hedonic tone of an odor,is the degree to which an odor is perceived as pleasant or unpleasant.Hedonic judgments include both a category judgment (pleasant,neutral, unpleasant) and a magnitude judgment (very unpleasant,slightly pleasant).Important questions areWhat is the minimum concentration of odorant that can bedetected?How does perceived odor magnitude grow with concentrationabove the threshold?No universal method has been accepted to measure ei

copyright@ 2008-2019 麦多课文库(www.mydoc123.com)网站版权所有
备案/许可证编号:苏ICP备17064731号-1