ImageVerifierCode 换一换
格式:DOC , 页数:11 ,大小:51KB ,
资源ID:472136      下载积分:2000 积分
快捷下载
登录下载
邮箱/手机:
温馨提示:
如需开发票,请勿充值!快捷下载时,用户名和密码都是您填写的邮箱或者手机号,方便查询和重复下载(系统自动生成)。
如填写123,账号就是123,密码也是123。
特别说明:
请自助下载,系统不会自动发送文件的哦; 如果您已付费,想二次下载,请登录后访问:我的下载记录
支付方式: 支付宝扫码支付 微信扫码支付   
注意:如需开发票,请勿充值!
验证码:   换一换

加入VIP,免费下载
 

温馨提示:由于个人手机设置不同,如果发现不能下载,请复制以下地址【http://www.mydoc123.com/d-472136.html】到电脑端继续下载(重复下载不扣费)。

已注册用户请登录:
账号:
密码:
验证码:   换一换
  忘记密码?
三方登录: 微信登录  

下载须知

1: 本站所有资源如无特殊说明,都需要本地电脑安装OFFICE2007和PDF阅读器。
2: 试题试卷类文档,如果标题没有明确说明有答案则都视为没有答案,请知晓。
3: 文件的所有权益归上传用户所有。
4. 未经权益所有人同意不得将文件中的内容挪作商业或盈利用途。
5. 本站仅提供交流平台,并不能对任何下载内容负责。
6. 下载文件中如有侵权或不适当内容,请与我们联系,我们立即纠正。
7. 本站不保证下载资源的准确性、安全性和完整性, 同时也不承担用户因使用这些下载资源对自己和他人造成任何形式的伤害或损失。

版权提示 | 免责声明

本文([外语类试卷]专业英语八级(阅读)模拟试卷122及答案与解析.doc)为本站会员(rimleave225)主动上传,麦多课文库仅提供信息存储空间,仅对用户上传内容的表现方式做保护处理,对上载内容本身不做任何修改或编辑。 若此文所含内容侵犯了您的版权或隐私,请立即通知麦多课文库(发送邮件至master@mydoc123.com或直接QQ联系客服),我们立即给予删除!

[外语类试卷]专业英语八级(阅读)模拟试卷122及答案与解析.doc

1、专业英语八级(阅读)模拟试卷 122及答案与解析 SECTION A MULTIPLE-CHOICE QUESTIONS In this section there are several passages followed by fourteen multiple-choice questions. For each multiple-choice question, there are four suggested answers marked A , B, C and D. Choose the one that you think is the best answer. 0 When

2、the Viaduct de Millau opened in the south of France in 2004, this tallest bridge in the world won worldwide compliments. German newspapers described how it “floated above the clouds“ with “elegance and lightness“ and “breathtaking“ beauty. In France, papers praised the “immense concrete giant“. Was

3、it mere coincidence that the Germans saw beauty where the French saw heft and power? Lera Boroditsky thinks not. A psychologist at Stanford University, she has long been intrigued by an age-old question whose modern form dates to 1956, when linguist Benjamin Lee Whorf asked whether the language we s

4、peak shapes the way we think and see the world. If so, then language is not merely a means of expressing thought, but a constraint on it, too. Although philosophers, anthropologists, and others have weighed in, with most concluding that language does not shape thought in any significant way, the fie

5、ld has been notable for a distressing lack of empiricismas in testable hypotheses and actual data. Thats where Boroditsky comes in. In a series of clever experiments guided by pointed questions, she is amassing evidence that, yes, language shapes thought. The effect is powerful enough, she says, tha

6、t “the private mental lives of speakers of different languages may differ dramatically,“ not only when they are thinking in order to speak, “but in all manner of cognitive tasks,“ including basic sensory perception. “Even a small fluke of grammar“the gender of nouns“can have an effect on how people

7、think about things in the world,“ she says. As in that bridge, in German, the noun for bridge, Brucke, is feminine. In French, pont is masculine. German speakers saw female features; French speakers, masculine ones. Similarly, Germans describe keys (Schlussel) with words such as hard, heavy, jagged,

8、 and metal, while to Spaniards keys (Haves) are golden, intricate, little, and lovely. Guess which language interprets key as masculine and which as feminine? Language even shapes what we see. People have a better memory for colors if different shades have distinct namesnot Englishs light blue and d

9、ark blue, for instance, but Russians goluboy and sinly. Skeptics of the language-shapes-thought claim have argued that thats a trivial finding, showing only that people remember what they saw in both a visual form and a verbal one, but not proving that they actually see the hues differently. In an i

10、ngenious experiment, however, Boroditsky and colleagues showed volunteers three color swatches and asked them which of the bottom two was the same as the top one. Native Russian speakers were faster than English speakers when the colors had distinct names, suggesting that having a name for something

11、 allows you to perceive it more sharply. Similarly, Korean uses one word for “in“ when one object is in another snugly (a letter in an envelope), and a different one when an object is in something loosely (an apple in a bowl). Sure enough, Korean adults are better than English speakers at distinguis

12、hing tight fit from loose fit. In Australia, the Aboriginal Kuuk Thaayorre use compass directions for every spatial cue rather than right or left, leading to locutions such as “there is an ant on your southeast leg. “ The Kuuk Thaayorre are also much more skillful than English speakers at dead recko

13、ning, even in unfamiliar surroundings or strange buildings. Their language “equips them to perform navigational feats once thought beyond human capabilities,“ Boroditsky wrote on Edge.org. Science has only scratched the surface of how language affects thought. In Russian, verb forms indicate whether

14、 the action was completed or notas in “she ate (and finished)the pizza.“ In Turkish, verbs indicate whether the action was observed or merely rumored. Boroditsky would love to run an experiment testing whether native Russian speakers are better than others at noticing if an action is completed, and

15、if Turks have a heightened sensitivity to fact versus hearsay. Similarly, while English says “she broke the bowl,“ even if it smashed accidentally (she dropped something on it, say), Spanish and Japanese describe the same event more like “the bowl broke itself. “ When we show people video of the sam

16、e event,“ says Boroditsky, “ English speakers remember who was to blame even in an accident, but Spanish and Japanese speakers remember it less well than they do intentional actions. It raises questions about whether language affects even something as basic as how we construct our ideas of causality

17、. “ 1 In the first paragraph, the author introduces his topic by_. ( A) explaining a phenomenon ( B) justifying an assumption ( C) posing a contrast ( D) making a comparison 2 Lera Boroditsky most probably holds the viewpoint that_. ( A) language expresses thought ( B) language constrains thought (

18、C) language determines thought ( D) language and thought interact with each other 3 Which of the following statements is true about the languages mentioned in the passage? ( A) Both the nouns for bridge and key are feminine in German. ( B) The language of the Aboriginal Kuuk Thaayorre is really help

19、ful for sailing. ( C) Korean has a larger vocabulary than English in describing colors. ( D) Whether an action is completed or not is best shown in Spanish. 4 The author uses the following ways to develop paragraphs EXCEPT_. ( A) cause and effect ( B) deduction and induction ( C) explanation ( D) de

20、finition 4 What would the holidays be without lots of tiny twinkling lights? Less colorful and festivebut also a lot safer. From living rooms to front porches across the country, homeowners are stringing millions of lights on Christmas trees or eaves and decorating their windowsills with electric, b

21、attery-operated or traditional candles. But according to the federal Consumer Product Safety Commission, too many are doing so with little regard to the hazards. Last holiday season there were about 200 Christmas tree fires in American homes, caused primarily by faulty lights and resulting in 10 dea

22、ths and more than $10 million in property loss, the Commission says. Another 14,000 house fires are started yearly by misplaced or mishandled flame candles, causing 170 deaths and $350 million in property loss. And about 10,000 people are treated at emergency rooms for injuries from falls, cuts or s

23、hocks while hanging lights or decorations. The biggest causes of holiday fires are “candles and live trees“, said Kim Dulic, a Commission spokeswoman. The agency recommends battery-operated candles instead of real or electric, she said, along with fire-resistant artificial treesor fresh well-watered

24、 trees. A cut tree is fresh, she said, if the bottom of its trunk is sticky with resin and its needles are hard to pull and dont break when bent. It is too dry if it sheds a shower of needles when bounced on the ground. A harvested tree should be cut about a half inch from the bottom and put in wate

25、r within no more than three to six hours, said Rick Dungey, the public relations manager of the National Christmas Tree Association, in Chesterfield, Mo. “If you wait any longer, air molecules get in the trunk and they prevent the tree from siphoning water,“ Mr. Dungey said, adding that people shoul

26、d water often and never let the water go below the cut end. Once a Christmas tree dries out, it is an accident waiting to happen, said Lorraine Carli, the communications vice president of the National Fire Protection Association, in Quincy, Mass. If ignited, it can be engulfed in seconds. The most c

27、ommon cause is electricaleither an overused electrical system or faulty wiring. Brett Brenner, the president of the Electrical Safety Foundation International (ESFI), in Rosslyn, Va., said homeowners should make yearly inspections. “Cracked sockets, frayed or bare wires and loose connections can cau

28、se a serious shock or start a fire,“ he said. Use no more than one extension cord per socket, and string no more than three sets of lights together. Wires should not run under carpets or through windows or doors. He said outdoor outlets should be protected by a ground fault circuit interruptera brea

29、ker that trips with any interruption or problem with the ground wire. (An interrupter usually needs to be installed when an outlet is near or exposed to water; it generally costs less than $10.) John Drengenberg, the consumer affairs director of Underwriters Laboratories, the testing group in Northb

30、rook, III. , said that if lights are certified for indoors only, they must not be used outside; those certified for outdoors, however, can be used inside. No matter the kind, he said, if the bulbs are the screw-in type, there should be no more than 50 per outlet. Outdoor lights, he said, should be h

31、ung with plastic clip-on hangers, not metal nails or staples, which can pierce insulation and cause a short. And what about those who dont take down their outdoor lights until the wisteria is in bloom in May? “You should never leave lights up all year round,“ Mr. Drengenberg said. “Theyre not design

32、ed for year-round use.“ 5 Which of the following is NOT mentioned in the passage as a cause of the holiday hazards? ( A) Accidents during decoration. ( B) Poor quality of bubbles. ( C) Careless handling of candles. ( D) Problematic management of lights. 6 According to the passage, what is the best c

33、hoice of Christmas trees? ( A) A real tree that is soaked in water at the shop. ( B) A real tree whose needles dont break when bent. ( C) An artificial tree with delicate craftsmanship. ( D) An artificial tree that wont be engulfed immediately. 7 It can be inferred from Paragraph Five that_. ( A) th

34、e ESFI inspects household electrical system annually ( B) electrical devices for outdoor use are not expensive ( C) homeowners do not have the particular electrical knowledge ( D) an overloaded electrical system or faulty wiring may lead to disasters 8 Which of the following is NOT in accordance wit

35、h Mr. Drengenbergs suggestion? ( A) Never use outdoor lights that are certified for indoor use. ( B) Put exactly 50 screw-in type bulbs to each outlet. ( C) Take off the outdoor lights after the Christmas season is over. ( D) Avoid metal nails or staples when putting on the outdoor lights. 8 We all

36、know that emotions originate in the brain. But we usually talk about our emotions coming from our hearts. If someone you know doesnt give up easily, you might say, “Hes got a lot of heart.“ Not every culture would agreefor instance, when Italians want to say someone has heart, they say instead, “Ha

37、fegato“ :“He has liver. “ But what about bad emotions? When you feel so sad or so angry that your heart “aches“, could it actually be true? Two new studies add support to the theory that, yes, what goes on in your mind can break your heart. In the first study, just published in the Journal of the Am

38、erican College of Cardiology (JACC), a team of eight researchers looking at more than 63,000 women who were participants in the ongoing Nurses Health Study, found that those who reported basic symptoms of depression (like feeling down and incapable of happiness) had a higher-than-normal risk of coro

39、nary heart disease. And women who were clinically depressed were more than twice as likely as other women to suffer sudden cardiac death. None of the participants had heart problems at the studys outset, but nearly 8% had symptoms of depression. The researchers theorize that depression might have so

40、me direct physiological impact on the heartlike causing it to work harder in the face of stress. The study also found that the more depressed women were, the more likely they were to smoke cigarettes or have high blood pressure and diabetesnot exactly heart-healthy conditions. Or it may be that the

41、antidepressants prescribed to treat those with mood problems were associated with heart ailments; in the study, sudden cardiac death was linked more strongly with antidepressant use than with womens symptoms of depression. The antidepressant theory is just thata theory. It could be that the antidepr

42、essant takers in the study were simply the most depressed. But if the theory is substantiated by further research, it would add to a growing body of evidence suggesting that antidepressants carry a high risk (particularly for teenagers) when weighed against the drugs still uncertain benefits. Scient

43、ists have already shown that antidepressants are a bad idea for those about to undergo coronary artery bypass surgery. No one is sure exactly how depression hurts the heart, and one plausible explanation is that the train runs in the opposite directiona damaged heart and its consequent stress on the

44、 body might activate, somehow, genes or other physiological changes that contribute to depression. But another new paper, also published in the JACC, lends credit to the idea that it is our moods that work on our hearts and not the other way around. In this paper, researchers from University College

45、 London reviewed the findings of 39 previously published articles and found that men who are angry and hostile are significantly more likely to have a cardiac event than those who arent. That may sound unsurprisingwe all know that anger can stress your heart. But its important to note the difference

46、 between aggression and just being aggressive. Previous studies have found that so-called type Asthose who are driven, competitive and obsessed with deadlinesare not more likely to experience heart disease. In other words, your type A co-workers who are annoyingly ambitious and dutiful are no more l

47、ikely to have a heart attack than you are. Rather, its the seething, angry types with underlying hostility who are the ticking time bombs. Anger, it turns out, is physiologically toxic. The authors of the second paper offer the standard theories about how an angry emotion translates to a physical he

48、art attack: angry people have a harder time sleeping; they take prescribed drugs less often; they eat worse, exercise less, smoke more and are fatter. These things add up: compared with the good-humored, those who were angry and hostilebut had no signs of heart problems at the outsetended up with a

49、19% higher risk of developing coronary heart disease, according to the University College London paper. The two studies reify gender stereotypes: women get their hearts broken through sadness; men “break“ their hearts (via heart attack) through anger. But both studies suggest that men and women have a common interest in understanding that some causes of cardiac diseasepoor diet or lack of exercise or bad sleep habitsmay have a precipitating cause themselves. Whether male or female, letting yourself get overwhel

copyright@ 2008-2019 麦多课文库(www.mydoc123.com)网站版权所有
备案/许可证编号:苏ICP备17064731号-1